Language, Power and Pedagogy Chapter 1: Issues and Contexts



Download 84.06 Kb.
Page1/2
Date29.01.2017
Size84.06 Kb.
#11098
  1   2
Language, Power and Pedagogy

 Chapter 1: Issues and Contexts 

Jim Cummins 

Komotini, Greece, October 2, 1999: Power Relations Past and Present

My feelings yesterday were almost surreal as I listened to Howard Smith of the University of Texas at San Antonio describe the history of educational oppression experienced by African American children in the United States. What was I doing sitting in an auditorium in the province of Thrace, near Turkey, at a conference focused on the education of the Muslim minorities in Greece, listening to the ugly history of racism on the other side of the globe, and how its residues still persist?

Today, I listened to Daphna Bassewitch Ginzburg and Anwar Dawod from Israel present Jewish and Palestinian perspectives on two different attempts to heal the wounds of the past (and present) through education. Both described settings at the preschool (Daphna) and elementary school (Anwar) where Jewish and Palestinian children are being educated together in the same classrooms, with the same curriculum, and using both Arabic and Hebrew as languages of instruction. I was struck by the courage of educators in these schools who saw education as a means of transforming the future rather than reproducing the past.

Maria del Socorro Leandro, Director of Bilingual Programs in the Edgewood School District in San Antonio, Texas, also spoke of the two-way bilingual immersion program in Edgewood that brings together English-dominant and Spanish-dominant students in the same classrooms with the goal of promoting fluent bilingualism and biliteracy for both groups of students. Here again was testimony about reclaiming dignity and voice from a people who had experienced subordination for more than 150 years. Educators who remembered the very recent past, up to the late 1960s, when Mexican-Americans were treated as an inferior species by all institutions of society, were using the power of language and education to repudiate that past and evoke a very different future.

Too often, the conflicts of the past pervade the classrooms of the present. This was illustrated vividly when Anna Frangoudaki and Thalia Dragonas of the University of Athens, organizers of the conference and directors of the project on the education of the Muslim minorities, spoke yesterday about the history of education in Thrace and the goals of their project. The Turkish-speaking population of Thrace became Greek citizens in May 1920 when western Thrace became part of Greece. The 1923 Treaty of Lausanne, which was referred to many times during the conference, still governs the education of the Muslim minorities in Greece. It provides for a separate education for the Muslim minorities with Turkish textbooks to be developed by the Turkish government. However, use of these textbooks is subject to approval by the Greek government. The curriculum in the Muslim minority schools is divided in two: half of the subjects are taught by Greek teachers and half by Turkish teachers. The school principal comes from the minority group and the vice-principal from the majority. The vast majority (99%) of the Muslim minority attend these segregated schools and have no contact with non-Muslim students until at least the age of 12 when they finish elementary school. More than 7,000 students attend 300 minority schools, many of them in isolated mountainous areas.

The situation is complicated by the fact that among the Muslim minorities are small groups of Roma communities speaking the Roma language as well as Pomak communities who speak a Slavic dialect related to Bulgarian. Although their language is not Turkish, their education is viewed by the Turkish minority and by the Turkish government as being regulated by the Treaty of Lausanne as a result of their Muslim religion. Originally, this interpretation of the Treaty was also encouraged by the Greek government. Consequently, these groups attend the Turkish/Greek medium minority schools and learn through two languages which are not their mother tongues. The mutual suspicion that has characterized relations with the Greek government and Greek communities of Thrace, together with pressure from the more powerful Turkish minority, has resulted in a bizarre situation where many Pomaks deny that they speak a different language and claim Turkish as their home language.

For the past 70 years, it seems that the Muslim minority children in Thrace have been pawns to be sacrificed in a struggle for historical righteousness. Turkish textbooks prepared by the Turkish government have been rejected by the Greek government on the grounds that they were full of anti-Greek propaganda. Turkish textbooks prepared in the early 1990s by the Greek government were burned by the Turkish community because they violated the Treaty of Lausanne. Many Greek teachers have approached their duty of “teaching the enemy” with a feeling of hostility to the language, culture, and traditions that children bring to school. Their expectations for student success have been extremely low and thus it was not difficult to rationalize the minimal literacy skills attained by the Muslim children in Greek (and even Turkish) as being due to their inherent inferiority. Contact between Greek and Muslim teachers in the school has tended to be minimal and characterized by the same hostility and suspicion.

The project that Anna Frangoudaki and Thalia Dragonas spoke about was funded by the European Union and has initiated extensive professional development for teachers in the minority schools over the previous two years. It has also developed attractive and culturally-sensitive teaching materials for use in teaching during the Greek part of the day. Not the least of its achievements has been to bring Greek and Muslim teachers into constructive dialogue, perhaps for the first time ever, regarding ways of improving the education of minority children. This conference itself seemed to represent a huge step forward. It was endorsed by both the “majority” and “minority” Primary Schools Teachers Associations in Thrace and the auditorium was filled with teachers from both communities,. The droning repetitious discourses of past hatreds were at least temporarily set aside, although their echoes could occasionally be heard in the reflections and recommendations of participants.

We started this morning at 9.30 and continued through the day until almost 9.00 in the evening. At the end of the conference, the auditorium was almost as packed as it was earlier in the day. My own intellectual and physical stamina exhausted, I remember thinking that I could imagine few settings in North America where educators or any other profession would give up their weekend for almost 12 straight hours of lectures and often passionate discussion. The devastating earthquakes that had struck both Turkey and Greece in the weeks prior to the conference, and the mutual assistance given by each country to the other, perhaps had contributed to shaking up old ways of thinking. Expansion of imaginative horizons could perhaps also be read into the rapt attention paid to accounts from both the United States and Israel of similarly conflictual and oppressive social relations, and the possibility that educators could transform these coercive social relations into collaborative ones. Even if this collaborative transformation were to occur only within the microcosm of the school, or in the interactions of individual educators with their students, it would nevertheless constitute what Noam Chomsky (1987) called “the threat of a good example.” When schools and individual educators refuse to play their preordained part in the social order, education becomes dangerous. The discourses of national and religious identity, and the historical myths that sustain them, risk implosion when contact and dialogue replace isolation and monologue. When two languages are used in the school to affirm the experiences and cultures of the students and communities who speak those languages, this in itself challenges the discourse of superiority and devaluation that characterizes social relations between these communities in the wider society.

To create a future we need to rupture the past.

Northern Ireland comes to mind and the half-truths of history into which I was socialized as a child.

It is not surprising to hear Thalia Dragonas speak of the change in attitudes towards the project among some officials in the Greek Ministry of Education—initial support and enthusiasm have been replaced by ambivalence and concern. Enemies have their place—they are essential to national identity. If education transforms enemies into colleagues, is it serving the interests of our society or undermining its strength?

Ironically, the Muslim children in Thrace have received a bilingual education for the past 70 years, illustrating the fact that language of instruction itself is only surface structure. Coercive power relations can be expressed as effectively through two languages as through one. Change in the deep structure will come only when educators walk into their classrooms burdened not by the anger of the past and the disdain of the present, but with their own identities focused on transforming the social futures towards which their students are traveling.

Victims and victimizers: Turkish minority children in Thrace caught in the crossfire of historical antagonisms, but at least with access to mother tongue instruction (however inadequate) and no prohibition on private use of Turkish; Kurdish children in Turkey still denied access to mother tongue instruction in any form and restrictions even on private use of Kurdish:

The only ban on the Kurdish language that has been lifted [by new legislation passed in 1991] is that on private use, provided it does not fall under the other paragraphs. Thus, Kurds are now allowed to speak Kurdish in their homes and sing Kurdish love songs in their gardens, but if a Kurdish child complains to a parent in a private garden, while picking beans, about not being allowed to speak Kurdish during the breaks in school, this act is still a terrorist crime. (Hassanpour, Skutnabb-Kangas, & Chyet, 1996: 371)

Earlier today, during one of the breaks, I spoke with a young man who was teaching in a Pomak village. He had come from another part of Greece and had not been socialized into the immediacy of hostile relations between Christians and Muslims that many Greeks in Thrace had experienced. He described his frustration at his inability to connect with his students early in the year. Teaching the prescribed curriculum was going nowhere. Things began to change when he discarded the curriculum and asked his students to teach him some of their language. In my terms, he moved from transmission of a prescribed curriculum rooted in a coercive power structure to an attempt to establish genuine human relationships with his students and their parents. His efforts were so successful that the community assumed that he must be a Muslim. When he demonstrated to them that he was in fact Christian (by showing them the cross he carried on a chain around his neck), the community was angry and rejected him. The teacher contemplated resigning and leaving the community but after further discussion the community asked him to stay.

This teacher’s experience brought to mind a quotation from Oscar Wilde which suggests that educators have both the responsibility, and the opportunity, to refuse complicity in the punishments inflicted by society on children, as demonstrated by this Greek teacher in a remote area of Thrace:

A child can understand a punishment inflicted by an individual, such as a parent or guardian, and bear it with a certain amount of acquiescence. What it cannot understand is a punishment inflicted by society. <1>


 

Tokyo, August 1-5, 1999: Measured Words and Dissenting Voices

Several items in the media caught my eye on this the first day of the International Applied Linguistics (AILA) Congress. An article in the Airport Limousine magazine which I had picked up the previous day en route from Narita airport was entitled “Japan’s Baby Bust: Young and Old Alike Feeling the Pinch” (Odani, 1999). It outlined the mostly bleak economic consequences of the drop in Japan’s birthrate to 1.4 births per woman, far below the 2.1 births required to sustain the population at its current level. Rapid constriction in all levels of education is predicted as fewer young people come to school and go to university. Without a dramatic increase in fertility (which is highly unlikely) or massive increases in immigration (which is currently negligible—foreigners account for only 1% of the Japanese population), the economy will shrink and make it difficult to maintain social services to the rapidly increasing elderly population (aged 65+) which will make up 20% of Japan’s population by 2005. Two days later, the Asahi Evening News elaborated on these trends (Kristof, 1999), pointing out that the working-age population of Japan will drop by about 650,000 a year over the next 50 years.

Dramatically increase immigration or face economic decline? Stark choices for a nation accustomed to viewing itself as homogenous and proud of it (leaving aside some blips on the homogeneity screen resulting from groups such as the Ainu, Koreans, and Burakumin [Maher & Yashiro, 1995]). To increase immigration in such a way that immigrants would want to settle and boost the population would entail significant social changes: reduce widespread discrimination against foreigners, implement effective Japanese L2 language learning programs, and possibly bilingual education in immigrant languages, and generally adjust a social and educational system to promote equity and academic advancement for second language learners. Not simple to do, or even to contemplate, because we are talking about fundamental changes to the culture and power structure of the society.

These issues are just beginning to appear on the horizon of public consciousness and, to its credit, the Japanese government has initiated research to address the educational issues faced by the inevitability of increasing diversity. Preliminary results from a large-scale study involving approximately 9,000 teachers, 800 parents, and 1,000 children from Portuguese-, Chinese-, Spanish-, and Vietnamese-speaking backgrounds led by Professor Suzuki Nishihara of Tokyo Women’s Christian University were reported at the AILA Congress. Among the findings reported by Professor Toshio Okazaki (1999) is a significant positive relationship between parental attitudes favoring active maintenance of their children’s home language and both L1 maintenance and L2 acquisition. A positive relationship or interdependence between L1 and L2 emerges after the L1 has reached a certain level of development. In other words, contrary to the views of many Japanese (and North American) educators, active promotion of the first language in the home appears to benefit not only development of L1 but also the L2.


The importance for families of L1 maintenance and the challenges involved in achieving this goal were apparent in a news clip in the August 1 Asahi Evening News. This article reported the retirement of the Buffalo Sabres’ Dominik Hasek, five-time choice as the National Hockey League’s best goalie, and quoted him as follows:

We want our kids to go back to the Czech Republic and share a Czech background. Every year he (9-year-old son) has more problems to speak Czech. ...The longer we stay in the United States, the harder it will be for our kids. (Asahi Evening News, August 1, 1999: 5)

Obviously, affluence and privilege alone can’t buy L1 maintenance in the face of the massive power of the dominant language in the environment.

I frequently hear sad anecdotes from international students enrolled in the University of Toronto about how their elementary school children are rejecting the home language and culture. After just two years in Canada, many children refuse to use the first language in the home and want to anglicize their names in order to belong to the culture of the school and peer group. I very rarely hear stories of how teachers communicate strong affirmative messages to students about the value of knowing additional languages. In the vacuum created by the absence of any proactive validation of their linguistic talents and accomplishments, bilingual students’ identities become infested with shame. A psychological phenomenon, to be sure; but also sociological. Pre-service teacher education programs across North America typically regard knowledge about linguistic and cultural diversity as appropriate for “additional qualification” courses rather than as part of the core knowledge base that all teachers should possess. As the discussions about assessment and pedagogy in Chapters 6 and 10 make clear, the contradictions that derive from viewing the generic student as monolingual, monocultural, white, middle-class, and heterosexual are becoming embarrassingly evident.

Dominik Hasek’s predicament, and that of millions of other minority language parents around the globe, reminds me of the incident that happened a few years ago in Amarillo, Texas, where State District Judge, Samuel Kiser, ordered a bilingual Mexican-American mother (Marta Laureano) involved in a child custody dispute to refrain from speaking Spanish to her daughter. If you are from a group that has historically been subordinated rather than a Hockey superstar, even the home is not a safe haven for the mother tongue. The judge told Laureano that she was “abusing” her five-year old daughter by speaking Spanish to her and ordered Laureano to speak only English at home. The father of the child, Timothy Garcia, who was seeking unsupervised visitation rights with his daughter, had complained that she was not proficient in English. As reported in Maclean's magazine (September 11, 1995: 13):

In court, Kiser told Laureano that she was relegating her daughter "to the position of housemaid." After a public outcry, Kiser backed down - a little. He apologized to housekeepers everywhere, "since we entrust our personal possessions and our families' welfare to these hardworking people." But otherwise, Kiser stood by his statements. Excerpts from his comments:

"If she starts first grade with the other children and cannot even speak the language that the teachers and others speak, and she's a full-blooded American citizen, you're abusing that child and you're relegating her to the position of housemaid. Now, get this straight: you start speaking English to that child, because if she doesn't do good [sic] in school, then I can remove her because it's not in her best interest to be ignorant.

"You are real big about talking about what's best for your daughter, but you won't even teach a five-year-old child how to speak English. And then you expect her to go off to school and educate herself and be able to learn how to make a living. Now that is bordering on abuse."

Despite Judge Kiser’s ruling, few in Texas would contemplate imposing legal prohibitions on the use of Spanish outside of school (although its status within schools is not entirely secure). The situation of Mexican-Americans in Texas is a far cry from that of Kurds in Turkey. However, Judge Kiser inadvertently reminds us that only 30 years ago, any use of Spanish in the schools by teachers would result in a $100 fine, and students caught speaking Spanish would usually be physically punished or humiliated.

Is promoting bilingualism in the home child abuse, or is it child abuse to punish children for being bilingual? To what extent is it child abuse to send new teachers into classrooms (in multilingual cities such as Toronto, London, or New York) with minimal or no preparation on how to teach academic content to students who are in the process of learning English and whose cultural background differs significantly from that assumed by all of the structures of schooling (e.g. curriculum, assessment, and teacher preparation)?

I wonder if Judge Kiser would be interested in reading about the multilingual accomplishments of developmentally disabled adolescents in Kenya in an article written in 1996 by Jamie Candelaria-Greene, a special educator and student teacher supervisor from California. Candelaria-Greene documents the fact that these mentally handicapped students attending Jacaranda School in Nairobi

were speaking an average of three languages at similar fluency rates. ... Thus, students spoke English as a second or third language as well as they might speak Kiswahili, Gujerati, or Kikuyu. As an instructor in both countries, I found that the Kenyan students, with Down Syndrome for example, demonstrated receptive and expressive language proficiency in their third language (English) equivalent to that of the US monolingual English students with Down Syndrome. (1996: 550)

In rural areas, the children’s L1 (tribal language) would be used in the home and for the development of literacy skills in lower elementary classes. Kiswahili (the national language) would be used for initial instruction in lower elementary classes in mixed language areas where it serves as the lingua franca. English (the official language) is used for academic instruction from upper elementary on and there is also exposure through television and other media outlets. 

Candelaria-Greene contrasts the favorable multilingual environment for children in Kenya with the fact that special education programming for similar children in the U.S. is usually through English only. She notes that “school professionals know little to nothing about their [limited English proficient] students’ language use patterns in the home communities” (1996: 560). Judge Kiser might be interested to know that “not once in two years [in Kenya] did I hear anyone blame academic failure or inappropriate behavior on the fact that a student’s family spoke a second language at home or that the student came from another tribe” (p. 560). She goes on to conclude that where “multilingualism and the various cultures they represent are valued by the society, and where there is a continued expectation and need for multilingualism to continue, students can and do manage second languages as well as they handle their first language, regardless of handicapping condition” (p. 560).


While in Tokyo, I read in a special August supplement to the Asahi Evening News, produced for the AILA congress, an article entitled Learning New Methods to Swim in English Ocean. The article discusses efforts to improve what many Japanese regard as the country’s dismal record in learning English. The article reports, for example, that despite its relative affluence, Japan ranks 150th out of 165 nations in TOEFL (Test of English as a Foreign Language) results and languishes near the bottom of Asian countries, performing little better than North Korea. The author concludes that “without a command of English, Japan is being left behind by the rest of the world” (Hiraoka, 1999: 3). Statements such as this might evoke a heated response from those who advocate a more critical perspective on the spread of English (see for example the articles in TESOL Quarterly, 33:3, Autumn 1999, edited by Alistair Pennycook, as well as Pennycook, 1998, and Phillipson, 1992, 1999).

Hiraoka raises the issue of what we mean by proficiency in English and how conceptions of proficiency relate to language pedagogy:

Today, two types of English are taught in Japan—entrance-exam English, and conversational English. When these two streams are brought together, the Japanese will at last be poised to join the world community. (1999: 3)

Again, I cringe at the assumption that knowledge of English is the passport to “join the world community.” However, the claim that what is assessed will determine what is taught and learned resonates with my perception of what is increasingly happening in ESL contexts in Britain and North America. As system-wide assessment schemes are introduced into more and more jurisdictions in the name of accountability, the tension is increasingly apparent between, on the one hand, performance assessments that reflect the full range of curriculum objectives (including critical thinking and creative writing) but which are costly and time-consuming to administer in a reliable way on a large-scale, and, on the other hand, standardized tests that are reliable and efficient to administer, but which typically reflect only a narrow band of easy-to-test curriculum objectives. Into this mix of unresolved issues, throw the rapid increase in linguistic diversity, with many students still in the process of learning the language of instruction and of testing, and you have a set of very high-stakes headaches for the czars of educational quality control.

The issues are surprisingly similar on both sides of the Atlantic, reflecting another aspect of globalization or, perhaps global homogenization. The May 28, 1999 Times Educational Supplement reported that refugee and immigrant children arriving in Britain with little or no English would be given “two years’ freedom from curriculum tests” (Jackson, 1999: 1) under proposals being considered by government ministers. These children would no longer be counted in their school’s test results thereby addressing an issue which was described as “a major cause of complaint for many inner-city schools” (1999: 1). The report goes on to note that there are half a million pupils in England who do not speak English as their first language and the proposed change “would give a considerable boost to schools’ placing in league tables” (1999: 1), the system of publishing the rankings of schools as a means of identifying “failing” schools and exerting pressures for improvement.

Exactly the same issue is being debated in both the United States and Canada (see Chapter 6) and in every case the one set of data that policy-makers want to hear nothing about is the fact that it typically takes immigrant children at least five years (often more) to catch up academically to native-speakers English. At the AILA conference, Professor Elana Shohamy of Tel Aviv University in Israel reported that in the Israeli context, a period of 7-9 years is typically required for immigrant students to catch up, a figure consistent with the range found in North American data (e.g. Cummins, 1981a; Hakuta, Butler, & Witt, 2000; Klesmer, 1994; Thomas & Collier, 1997). If a period of 5+ years is typically required to catch up, then delaying testing for two years, as proposed in the United Kingdom (UK), reduces the inequity only slightly. Including these students in the testing will still skew the results and undermine the whole accountability enterprise unless the data are disaggregated in intelligent ways. This intelligence has to date not been very evident in the North American context, where policy-makers have preferred to bury their heads in the sand rather than really come to terms with what I call in Chapter 6, “the awkward reality” of English language learning (ELL) students.

It would not be difficult to disaggregate the data according to a variety of criteria (e.g. poverty/socioeconomic status, proportion of ELL students, etc.) so that the quality of school instruction would emerge more clearly. In fact, a recent analysis in the Times Education Supplement does exactly this for the General Certificate of Secondary Education (GCSE) national school examination results at the secondary level. According to the report, “it shows huge variations in education authorities with apparently similar levels of deprivation” (Dean, 1999). Furthermore, some authorities with high poverty levels emerged as “winners” rather than “losers” when the data were disaggregated:

Arguably, the best-performing authority is the London borough of Tower Hamlets, where more than two-thirds of pupils are on free school meals, making it the most deprived area in the country on this measure. Though its GCSE score of 32.8 puts it well below the national average of 38.1, on the TES [Times Educational Supplement] analysis its pupils scored 7.3 GCSE points above what might be expected... (Dean, 1999)

The same kind of analysis could be carried out for the proportion of ELL students in different schools, weighted by length of residence in the country.

So where does the figure of 2 years come from as the criterion for including or excluding ELL students from national testing? Presumably from the “common sense” notion that this is how long it takes students to “learn English.” As the chapters in Part II of the present volume argue, this conception of the nature of English proficiency is either naive or perhaps cynical; it reflects typical time periods required to gain a reasonable degree of conversational fluency in English but not the length of time required to catch up to native speakers in academic aspects of English. This example, and many more that could be drawn from other contexts, illustrates how crucial it is for policy-makers and educators to have a clear conception of what they mean by “language proficiency.” In an era of widespread linguistic and cultural diversity, educational policies on curriculum and assessment that relegate considerations regarding diversity and language to “afterthought” or “footnote” status are likely to produce discriminatory instruction and utterly meaningless accountability data.


The Alice in Wonderland nature of much of the “accountability and standards” debate in North America was aptly pinpointed by well-known educational writer Gerald W. Bracey in an article in USA Today which appeared in early November 1999. Bracey (1999: 19A) points to the hypocrisy of the rhetoric of educational reform in light of the fact that politicians and policy-makers have shown minimal interest in addressing issues of child poverty which has a “devastating impact” on school performance:

Poor children get off to a bad start before they are born. Their mothers are likely to get prenatal care late, if at all, which can impair later intellectual functioning. They are more than three times as likely as nonpoor children to have stunted growth. They are about twice as likely to have physical and mental disabilities, and are seven times more likely to be abused or neglected. And they are more than three times more likely to die.

What these kids need are high standards, right? (1999: 19A)

Payne and Biddle (1999) have recently demonstrated the independent effects of school funding levels and child poverty on mathematics achievement in the United States. Together these variables accounted for 25% of the variance in achievement. Level of curriculum challenge (ranging from remedial to advanced algebra curriculum) was also significantly related to achievement. Payne and Biddle point out that despite continuous economic growth during the past decade, the child poverty level in the most affluent country in the world is still more than 20 percent, substantially higher than any other industrialized nation. They suggest a far more likely explanation for the relatively poor showing of U.S. schools in international comparisons than the “declining standards” usually invoked by politicians:

Since poorly funded schools and communities with high levels of poverty are very rare in other industrialized nations, education in America is uniquely handicapped because of the singular tolerance for large numbers of poorly funded schools and massive amounts of child poverty in our country. And as long as this tolerance continues, none of the present programs being touted for “reforming” American education—educational vouchers, “setting high standards,” “accountability” schemes, charter schools—are likely to improve America’s aggregate math achievement substantially. (1999: 12)

I find the educational standards debate (discussed in Chapter 6) particularly interesting because policy-makers and politicians are being forced to come to terms with the situation of ELL/bilingual students in order to implement their grand designs for higher standards (and often their own aspirations for political advancement). The blatant contradictions between the political rhetoric of higher standards and the tolerance for massive child poverty and hugely inequitable school funding exposes very clearly the discourse of coercive power relations.

These contradictions are also readily apparent in debates on bilingual education considered in the next section. The clear message being broadcast by the media in the United States is that bilingual education is a cause of further impoverishment for the poor but a potential source of further enrichment for the rich.


Download 84.06 Kb.

Share with your friends:
  1   2




The database is protected by copyright ©ua.originaldll.com 2024
send message

    Main page